HIV 884
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Association of HIV prevalence and concurrency of sexual partnerships in South Africa’s language groups: An ecological analysis

C Kenyon

STI/HIV Unit, Institute of Tropical Medicine, Antwerp, Belgium, and Division of Infectious Diseases and HIV Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Cape Town

C Kenyon, MB ChB, MA, MPH, PhD, FCP, FCP (Cert ID)


Corresponding author: C Kenyon (chriskenyon0@gmail.com)


Background. There is considerable variation in HIV prevalence between different language groups in South Africa (SA). Sexual partner concurrency has been linked to the spread of HIV, but its effect on differential HIV transmission within SA’s language groups has not been investigated quantitatively.

Objective. This ecological analysis was intended to explore the degree to which the variation in HIV prevalence according to language group can be explained by differential concurrency rates.

Method. Linear regression was used to assess the association between each language group’s HIV prevalence and four risk factors: the prevalence of concurrency, multiple sexual partners in the preceding year, circumcision, and condom utilisation.

Results. In multivariate analysis, only the point prevalence of concurrency remained associated with HIV prevalence.

Conclusion. There is evidence of a high prevalence of point concurrency in sexual partnerships in SA’s most HIV-affected language groups. Together with evidence that relatively small decreases in concurrency can lead to large declines in HIV incidence, this provides impetus for interventions to promote having only one sexual partner at a time.

S Afr J HIV Med 2013;14(1):25-28. DOI:10.7196/SAJHIVMED.884



Although adult HIV incidence in South Africa (SA) has fallen somewhat, it remains alarmingly high – between 1% and 2%.1 It is of great importance to ascertain what is driving this high incidence. One approach that has received little attention is to compare the potential risk factors for HIV in SA’s various language groups. Since HIV prevalence varies widely among these groups, this offers an opportunity to determine which population-level factors co-vary most closely with this prevalence. The objective of this analysis was to determine the manner in which HIV prevalence varies according to SA’s 11 major self-defined language groups, and to examine the ecological association of four risk factors (prevalence of concurrency, multiple partners in the preceding year, circumcision, and condom utilisation) with HIV prevalence in these groups.

Methods

Two nationally representative surveys were used for this study, namely the South African National HIV Prevalence, HIV Incidence, Behaviour and Communication Survey of 2008 (SABSSM III) and the National Communication Survey of 2009 (NCS 2009).2 , 3 In both surveys, respondents were asked to verify which main language they spoke at home; responses were coded into 11 identical language options (Table 1). The HIV prevalence (dependent variable) and risk factors (independent variables: prevalence of concurrency, multiple sexual partners in the preceding year, circumcision, and condom utilisation) were calculated for each language group.

HIV prevalence

The HIV prevalence of each language group (among individuals aged 16 - 55 years) was obtained from the SABBSSM III survey. 2 This was the third and most recent of the SABSSM surveys, which are the only nationally representative HIV sero-surveys of South Africans of all ages. The survey used a multi-stage stratified sampling approach. When correctly weighted to account for the complex sampling design and HIV testing non-response, the sample was representative of the population in SA for the main reporting domains of sex, age, race and province.2 Structured questionnaires were used to collect demographic, social and behavioural data. Dried blood-spot specimens were used for HIV testing using an algorithm that saw all samples initially being tested with an enzyme immuno-assay (Vironostika HIV Uni-Form II plus O, Biomerieux). Of 23 369 individuals, 20 826 (89.1%) completed the interviews and 15 031 (64.3%) agreed to provide blood for HIV testing. The mid-point of data collection was September 2008.

Risk factors

The four independent variables were derived from the NCS 20093 – a cross-sectional survey that utilised a multi-stage, stratified sampling approach (comprising three stages). Firstly, 400 primary sampling units (PSUs) were sampled using principles of probability proportional to size. PSUs comprised small areas from the 2001 National Census. The second and third stages, respectively, involved the selection of secondary sampling units or households, and the selection of one individual per household (aged 15 - 55 years) from eligible household members. The final sample comprised 9 728 individuals aged 16 - 55 years, who were representative of South Africans in this age band. The overall response rate was 58%. Data were collected between June and August 2009. See Johnson et al.3 for further details of the methodology and possible bias introduced by differential non-response. The four independent variables were defined as follows:

Point concurrency: The point prevalence of concurrency (i.e. having two or more overlapping sexual relationships) at the time of the survey was used as the indicator of concurrency, as this has been shown to best capture the effect thereof in increasing a sexual network’s connectivity and, hence, HIV transmissibility.4 , 5 For each language group, the point concurrency was determined by the percentage of persons who reported having two or more partners at the time of the survey. This variable was derived from the question: ‘How many sexual partners do you currently have?’

Multiple partners per year: defined as the proportion of respondents in each language group who reported having two or more sexual partners in the preceding 12 months.

Condom utilisation: defined as the proportion of respondents in each language group who reported using a condom the last time they had sexual intercourse.

Circumcision: defined as the proportion of male respondents who reported being circumcised (each male respondent was asked whether or not he was circumcised).

Statistical analyses

The HIV prevalence and independent variables were calculated for each self-defined language group using Stata version 12.0 (College Station, Texas, USA) and by applying the survey methodology to account for the multi-stage sampling strategies and varying non-response rates. Uni- and multivariate linear regression models were used to assess the association between the independent and dependent variables. All analyses were limited to sexually experienced individuals aged 16 - 55 years. The data were not age-standardised, as the differences in the age structure of each language group were relatively small (Table 1).

Table 1. Prevalence of HIV 2 and various risk factors 3 per language group among South Africans aged 16 - 55 years

Language

SABSSM III2


NCS 20093

N

Age

median (IQR)

HIV prevalence

% (95% CI)


N

Age

median (IQR)

Concurrency

% (95% CI)

Multiple partners per year

% (95% CI)

Circumcision

% (95% CI)

Condom utilisation

% (95% CI)

IsiZulu

1 646

28 (21 - 40)

28.8 (24.3 - 31.8)


1 973

29 (23 - 38)

8.9 (7.3 - 10.9)

16.8 (14.4 - 19.7)

23.5 (20.0 - 27.3)

50.5 (46.2 - 54.7)

IsiZhosa

1 497

28 (20 - 40)

21.6 (17.6 - 24.6)


1 351

28 (22 - 39)

4.9 (3.5 - 6.9)

11.6 (9.4 - 14.2)

76.6 (72.3 - 80.4)

45.8 (42.1 - 54.7)

IsiNdebele

105

28 (21 - 41)

20.6 (9.4 - 38.5)


191

27 (22 - 36)

6.1 (3.0 - 12.0)

9.4 (5.0 - 17.0)

68.0 (52.9 - 80.1)

44.4 (33.7 - 55.5)

IsiSwati

251

28 (19 - 41)

23.9 (18.1 - 30.0)


365

25 (21 - 34)

5.3 (3.6 - 7.6)

7.7 (5.3 - 10.9)

32.9 (19.5 - 49.8)

51.7 (42.7 - 60.6)

English

1 847

32 (21 - 43)

1.5 (0.8 - 2.6)


370

36 (27 - 44)

1.4 (0.5 - 3.9)

3.1 (1.4 - 6.9)

31.9 (22.3 - 43.4)

22.8 (16.6 - 30.3)

Afrikaans

2 568

33 (21 - 44)

2.5 (1.8 - 3.3)


1 228

36 (26 - 44)

1.2 (0.6 - 2.4)

4.3 (2.8 - 6.6)

14.8 (10.3 - 21.0)

21.3 (17.0 - 26.3)

Sesotho

783

29 (21 - 40)

20 (16.6 - 22.9)


946

31 (23 - 40)

4.6 (3.0 - 7.0)

13.2 (9.9 - 17.5)

49.2 (42.5 - 56.1)

44.5 (39.6 - 49.5)

Sepedi

808

29 (20 - 42)

16.6 (11.4 - 21.6)


797

26 (21 - 35)

5.4 (5.0 - 7.5)

12.8 (9.1 - 17.8)

79.5 (72.5 - 85.0)

54.0 (44.9 - 62.9)

Setswana

852

29 (20 - 41)

18.4 (13.6 - 22.7)


699

31 (24 - 40)

4.6 (2.8 - 7.5)

11.0 (7.5 - 15.9)

31.6 (23.4 - 41.2)

49.2 (43.9 - 54.5)

Tshivenda

143

27 (20 - 41)

8.1 (3.2 - 17.8)


232

28 (22 - 36)

3.8 (1.5 - 9.5)

10.9 (6.5 - 17.5)

89.2 (77.4 - 95.2)

45.6 (38.1 - 53.2)

Xitsonga

331

28 (21 - 38)

17.6 (10.6 - 26.3)


374

27 (22 - 36)

5.4 (2.3 - 11.9)

11.8 (7.4 - 18.3)

70.1 (55.4 - 82.4)

39.5 (30.0 - 49.8)

SABSSM III = South African National HIV Prevalence, HIV Incidence, Behaviour and Communication Survey of 2008; NCS 2009 = National Communication Survey of 2009; IQR = interquartile range; CI = confidence interval.


Results

Table 1 shows the variation in HIV prevalence between language groups, ranging from 1.5% (95% CI 0.8 - 2.6) to 28.8% (95% CI 24.3 - 31.8). These variations remained considerable upon analysis of the nine black language groups alone; ranging from 8.1% (95% CI 3.2 - 17.8) in Tshivenda speakers to 28.8% (95% CI 24.3 - 31.8) in isiZulu speakers.

Three risk factors were strongly associated with increased HIV prevalence per language group upon univariate analysis: multiple partners per year, point concurrency (Fig. 1) and lower condom utilisation rates (Table 2). Circumcision prevalence rates were not associated with HIV prevalence; however, this may have been driven by the effect of the English- and Afrikaans-speaking groups who had low rates of circumcision and HIV prevalence (Fig. 2). When the analysis was restricted to the nine black language groups, increasing circumcision rates were correlated with lower HIV prevalence rates (R2=0.48; p=0.04). In multivariate analysis, only point concurrency remained associated with HIV prevalence (β co-efficient=3.5; p=0.03) (Table 2).

Fig. 1. Association between HIV prevalence (derived from SABSSM III) and the point prevalence of concurrency (derived from NCS 2009) for 11 language groups in South Africa (R2 =-0.84; p< 0.001).[2,3]

Fig. 2. Association between HIV prevalence (derived from SABSSM III) and the prevalence of male circumcision (derived from NCS 2009), for 11 language groups in South Africa (R2 =-0.02; p=0.70 for all 11 language groups and R2 =0.48; p=0.04 when analysis restricted to the nine black language groups). [2,3]

Table 2. Univariate and multivariate linear regression analysis of the relationship between HIV prevalence per language group and risk factors2 , 3

Risk factor

Univariate


Multivariate

β co-efficient

R2

p -value


β co-efficient

p -value

Concurrency

3.79

0.84

0.0001


3.50

0.046

Multiple partners/year

2.04

0.55

0.0061


-0.23

0.949

Circumcision

0.04

0.02

0.8132


-0.02

0.427

Condom utilisation

0.61

0.58

0.0165


0.24

0.267


There was a high degree of overlap between language and self-reported ethnicity within the NCS 2009 sample. The proportion of coloureds, Indians and whites who spoke English or Afrikaans was 91.9%, 97.9% and 97.8%, respectively. The proportion of blacks who spoke English or Afrikaans as their home language was 1.6%. Omitting these individuals from the analyses made no difference to the results (data not shown). Moreover, it is possible that HIV prevalence may peak in different language groups at different times depending on the stage of the epidemic. To evaluate this, we repeated the analyses using the HIV prevalence rates from the 2002 and 2005 SABSSM surveys. The resultant difference to the results was negligible (data not shown).

Discussion

HIV prevalence is known to vary dramatically between South African language and racial groups.2 This heterogeneity offers a useful opportunity to examine the reasons underpinning the country’s generalised HIV epidemic. Great caution needs to exercised in the use of ethnic and racial categories in health research. This is especially the case in SA, where the uncritical use of racial categories in the apartheid era, combined with the concomitant lack of controlling for the effects of the widely divergent socio-economic conditions, served to exaggerate racial differentials in various health outcomes. 6-8 However, a wide range of evidence indicates that economic differences are not the predominant drivers of differential HIV spread according to racial group.9 Furthermore, it is important to explain the considerable differences in HIV prevalence between language groups among black South Africans.

There is a high degree of homophilous partnering (like-with-like) among self-defined language groups in sub-Saharan Africa10 and elsewhere.11 Sexual networks would therefore be expected to cluster and segregate to a considerable degree along these lines, as has been demonstrated empirically.10 , 11 These sexual networks may be, more or less, densely interconnected and these differences are believed by many,4 , 12 but not all, epidemiologists13 to be important in explaining differential HIV spread. Since network connectivity, as assessed by measures such as concurrency prevalence, is a network-level property, it is necessary and appropriate to investigate it at a network or ecological level.

A number of studies from SA, the USA and elsewhere have found that racial or ethnic variations in HIV prevalence are not explained by individual-level risk factors (e.g. multiple partners per year and lifetime number of sexual partners), but rather that network-level factors such as concurrency prevalence are important.4 , 14 , 15 This is commensurate with global reviews of sexual behaviour which have shown that the average number of lifetime sexual partners is, if anything, lower in countries with generalised HIV epidemics than in countries with low HIV prevalence rates such as those in Western Europe.16

In the data described here, the relationship between circumcision and HIV prevalence is interesting, especially considering the significant association within the black language groups. Circumcision cannot, however, explain the low HIV prevalence rates in the English and Afrikaans groups, as they have the lowest circumcision rates. This is mirrored globally. Eastern and Southern Africa have considerably higher circumcision rates than Latin America, and the non-Islamic countries in Asia and Europe, all of which have very low HIV prevalence rates. 17 , 18 Clearly, something else may be driving the higher HIV prevalence rates. The multivariate analyses presented here support findings from elsewhere which suggest that the degree of connectedness of the sexual network (here measured by point prevalence of concurrency) is playing a significant role in this regard.4 , 14 , 15 , 19 , 20

Study limitations

There are a number of weaknesses in this analysis, including the fact that the data for sexual behaviour and HIV prevalence were derived from different surveys. Both surveys were, however, conducted with nationally representative samples. The surveys were designed to provide representative data for the four racial groups in SA, but not for the eleven language groups. Ecological analyses, such as this one, assume a high degree of language group homophily as far as sexual partnering is concerned. This has been long been shown to be the case in the USA,11 but only recently so in SA.10 The data are derived from self-reported behaviour and circumcision statuses; however, these are prone to well-described biases.11 In particular, self-described circumcision has been shown to over-estimate circumcision prevalence.21 There is, however, no evidence to indicate that these biases vary between different language groups and, as such, they should not affect the validity of this study. Furthermore, ecological studies are susceptible to the ecological inference fallacy. This study, however, makes no inferences from the population to the individual level. Further work is necessary to evaluate whether partner concurrency is associated with an increased risk of HIV acquisition in prospective cohorts. Lastly, it is possible that the study’s results may have been confounded by unmeasured variables.

Conclusion

In summary, evidence is presented here of a high prevalence of point concurrency in sexual partnerships in SA’s most HIV-affected language groups. Other studies have found that these groups may be unaware of the dangers of concurrency.22 These results combined with the evidence that relatively small decreases in concurrency can lead to large declines in HIV incidence provide further impetus for interventions to promote having only one partner at a time.15 , 19 , 20


References
    1. UNAIDS. South Africa: Fact sheet. Geneva: UNAIDS, 2012. http://www.unaids.org/en/regionscountries/countries/southafrica/ (accessed 12 October 2012).

    1. UNAIDS. South Africa: Fact sheet. Geneva: UNAIDS, 2012. http://www.unaids.org/en/regionscountries/countries/southafrica/ (accessed 12 October 2012).

    2. Shisana O. South African National HIV Prevalence, Incidence, Behaviour and Communication Survey, 2008: A Turning Tide Among Teenagers? Cape Town: HSRC Press, 2009.

    2. Shisana O. South African National HIV Prevalence, Incidence, Behaviour and Communication Survey, 2008: A Turning Tide Among Teenagers? Cape Town: HSRC Press, 2009.

    3. Johnson S, Kincaid L, Laurence S, et al. Second National HIV Communication Survey, 2009. Pretoria: JHHESA, 2010.

    3. Johnson S, Kincaid L, Laurence S, et al. Second National HIV Communication Survey, 2009. Pretoria: JHHESA, 2010.

    4. Morris M, Epstein H, Wawer M. Timing is everything: International variations in historical sexual partnership concurrency and HIV prevalence. PLoS One 2010;5:e14092.

    4. Morris M, Epstein H, Wawer M. Timing is everything: International variations in historical sexual partnership concurrency and HIV prevalence. PLoS One 2010;5:e14092.

    5. Glynn JR, Dube A, Kayuni N, et al. Measuring concurrency: An empirical study of different methods in a large population-based survey and evaluation of the UNAIDS guidelines. AIDS 2012;26:977-985.

    5. Glynn JR, Dube A, Kayuni N, et al. Measuring concurrency: An empirical study of different methods in a large population-based survey and evaluation of the UNAIDS guidelines. AIDS 2012;26:977-985.

    6. Baldwin-Ragaven L, London L. An ambulance of the wrong colour: Health professionals, human rights and ethics in South Africa. Cape Town: Juta and Company Ltd, 1999.

    6. Baldwin-Ragaven L, London L. An ambulance of the wrong colour: Health professionals, human rights and ethics in South Africa. Cape Town: Juta and Company Ltd, 1999.

    7. Ncayiyana DJ. Racial profiling in medical research: What are we measuring? S Afr Med J 2007;97(12):1225-1226.

    7. Ncayiyana DJ. Racial profiling in medical research: What are we measuring? S Afr Med J 2007;97(12):1225-1226.

    8. Van Niekerk AA. Deliberating about race as a variable in biomedical research. S Afr Med J 2011;101(4):248-250.

    8. Van Niekerk AA. Deliberating about race as a variable in biomedical research. S Afr Med J 2011;101(4):248-250.

    9. Kenyon C. ‘Differential poverty rates are responsible for the racial differentials in HIV prevalence in South Africa’: An enduring and dangerous epidemiological urban legend? Southern African Journal of HIV Medicine 2010;11:22.

    9. Kenyon C. ‘Differential poverty rates are responsible for the racial differentials in HIV prevalence in South Africa’: An enduring and dangerous epidemiological urban legend? Southern African Journal of HIV Medicine 2010;11:22.

    10. Kenyon C, Colebunders R. Birds of a feather; homophily and sexual network structure in sub-Saharan Africa. Int J STD AIDS 2012 (in press).

    10. Kenyon C, Colebunders R. Birds of a feather; homophily and sexual network structure in sub-Saharan Africa. Int J STD AIDS 2012 (in press).

    11. Laumann EO. The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1994.

    11. Laumann EO. The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1994.

    12. Mah TL, Halperin DT. Concurrent sexual partnerships and the HIV epidemics in Africa: Evidence to move forward. AIDS Behav 2010;14:11-16.

    12. Mah TL, Halperin DT. Concurrent sexual partnerships and the HIV epidemics in Africa: Evidence to move forward. AIDS Behav 2010;14:11-16.

    13. Sawers L, Stillwaggon E. Concurrent sexual partnerships do not explain the HIV epidemics in Africa: A systematic review of the evidence. J Int AIDS Soc 2010;13:34.

    13. Sawers L, Stillwaggon E. Concurrent sexual partnerships do not explain the HIV epidemics in Africa: A systematic review of the evidence. J Int AIDS Soc 2010;13:34.

    14. Kenyon C, Dlamini S, Boulle A, et al. A network-level explanation for the differences in HIV prevalence in South Africa’s racial groups. Afr J AIDS Res 2009;8:243-254.

    14. Kenyon C, Dlamini S, Boulle A, et al. A network-level explanation for the differences in HIV prevalence in South Africa’s racial groups. Afr J AIDS Res 2009;8:243-254.

    15. Morris M, Kurth AE, Hamilton DT, et al. Concurrent partnerships and HIV prevalence disparities by race: Linking science and public health practice. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(6):1023-1031.

    15. Morris M, Kurth AE, Hamilton DT, et al. Concurrent partnerships and HIV prevalence disparities by race: Linking science and public health practice. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(6):1023-1031.

    16. Wellings K, Collumbien M, Slaymaker E, et al. Sexual behaviour in context: A global perspective. Lancet 2006;368(9548):1706-1728.

    16. Wellings K, Collumbien M, Slaymaker E, et al. Sexual behaviour in context: A global perspective. Lancet 2006;368(9548):1706-1728.

    17. Weiss H. Male Circumcision: Global Trends and Determinants of Prevalence, Safety, and Acceptability. Report No. 9291736333. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2008.

    17. Weiss H. Male Circumcision: Global Trends and Determinants of Prevalence, Safety, and Acceptability. Report No. 9291736333. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2008.

    18. UNAIDS. Report on the Global AIDS Epidemic. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2010.

    18. UNAIDS. Report on the Global AIDS Epidemic. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2010.

    19. Halperin DT, Mugurungi O, Hallett TB, et al. A surprising prevention success: Why did the HIV epidemic decline in Zimbabwe? PLoS Med 2011;8:e1000414.

    19. Halperin DT, Mugurungi O, Hallett TB, et al. A surprising prevention success: Why did the HIV epidemic decline in Zimbabwe? PLoS Med 2011;8:e1000414.

    20. Kirby D. Changes in sexual behaviour leading to the decline in the prevalence of HIV in Uganda: Confirmation from multiple sources of evidence. Sex Transm Dis 2008;84:ii35-ii41.

    20. Kirby D. Changes in sexual behaviour leading to the decline in the prevalence of HIV in Uganda: Confirmation from multiple sources of evidence. Sex Transm Dis 2008;84:ii35-ii41.

    21. Thomas AG, Tran BR, Cranston M, et al. Voluntary medical male circumcision: A cross-sectional study comparing circumcision self-report and physical examination findings in Lesotho. PLoS One 2011;6:e27561.

    21. Thomas AG, Tran BR, Cranston M, et al. Voluntary medical male circumcision: A cross-sectional study comparing circumcision self-report and physical examination findings in Lesotho. PLoS One 2011;6:e27561.

    22. Kenyon C, Zondo S, Badri M. Determinants of self-perceived HIV risk in young South Africans engaged in concurrent sexual relationship. Afr J Reprod Health 2010;14(3):171-181.

    22. Kenyon C, Zondo S, Badri M. Determinants of self-perceived HIV risk in young South Africans engaged in concurrent sexual relationship. Afr J Reprod Health 2010;14(3):171-181.





 

Crossref Citations

1. HIV epidemic drivers in South Africa: A model-based evaluation of factors accounting for inter-provincial differences in HIV prevalence and incidence trends
Leigh F. Johnson, Rob E. Dorrington, Haroon Moolla
Southern African Journal of HIV Medicine  vol: 18  issue: 1  year: 2017  
doi: 10.4102/sajhivmed.v18i1.695